What if Monet were shorter? | Pursuit by The University of Melbourne

2022-06-24 20:14:18 By : Mr. Kyle Chan

Altering the height of an artist’s eye level changes their perspective – but might this also alter the appeal of their paintings?

By Associate Professor Andrew Anderson, University of Melbourne

While visiting the Fitzwilliam Museum in Cambridge a few years ago, I happened upon the French Impressionist Claude Monet’s Rocks at Port Coton, The Lion Rock.

Painted during his 1886 visit to the French island of Belle-Île, the work depicts the famous Lion Rock, sitting in the water below a cliff whose top perfectly aligns with the distant horizon.

I didn’t like it. But I also had a good idea as to why I didn’t like it.

In 1999, influential neuroscientist Professor V. S. Ramachandran and American philosopher Professor William Hirstein proposed several principles they believed underpinned people’s aesthetic preferences in images.

One principle was that people prefer generic viewpoints, where the spatial relationship between objects is largely similar despite small changes in the viewer’s position.

For example, in the cartoon on the left in Figure 1 below, the foreground tree would appear somewhere between the peak and valley of the background hills even if the observer moved a little to the left or the right.

So, the general composition of the work is maintained despite small alterations in our viewpoint.

But if we look at the image on the right in Figure 1, the alignment between the foreground tree and the valley between the background hills would occur only from a single, specific viewpoint – so if the image were created with the observer moved a little to the left or the right, this alignment would be broken.

In general, generic viewpoints are more common, making the spatial coincidences arising from specific viewpoints relatively unlikely.

It is argued that these kind of spatial coincidences are seen by the brain as suspicious given their unlikely nature and, as a result, are unappealing.

In the case of foreground and background objects, it’s possible that the parts of the brain processing this visual information need to exert additional effort to ensure that a spatial coincidence is just that – a chance finding – rather than indicating the background and foreground are somehow physically connected.

In the case of the Monet image, the coincidence between the clifftop and the horizon likely makes my brain need a few extra moments to decide that the distant cliff isn’t physically continuous with the horizon, but merely appears so from this particular vantage point.

Over the 20 years since Ramachandran and Hirstein proposed that generic views are more aesthetically appealing, their work has been highly cited. Indeed, their paper is in Ramachandran’s top five most cited works, which is impressive given he is a particularly highly cited neuroscientist.

Despite this, experimental support for their idea is limited.

Given this – and prompted by my encounter with Monet’s Lion Rock – a team of students from the Department of Optometry and Vision Sciences and I set out to test Ramachandran and Hirstein’s proposal.

We had people view a range of image pairs containing both specific and generic viewpoints between foreground and background objects and we asked them to choose which image they liked more.

Our results found strong experimental support for the idea that people find generic viewpoints more appealing.

This includes when people are presented with a Monet image manipulated to give a more generic view (Figure 2). This would have been the result if Monet were (substantially) shorter – or, more realistically, if he had found a vantage point with a different elevation.

Of course, there is more to a beautiful work of art than simply the spatial relationship between foreground and background objects. Various factors influence what we find appealing and this is reflected in the substantial variation in responses between our experimental observers.

While our manipulations improve the overall appeal of Monet’s Lion Rock relative to the original, most people still find this manipulated work less appealing than many other works painted during his Belle-Île visit (Figure 3).

So really, the influence of the specific viewpoint in The Lion Rock is small compared to the sum of other factors influencing aesthetic appeal.

Indeed, Ramachandran and Hirstein outline a number of visual factors that they think influence what we find appealing – along with evolutionary rationales for why this might be so.

Using careful experiments to quantify how important these factors are might then allow us to better appreciate what non-visual factors – like cultural factors – affect what we find appealing.

Experiments like this might also help identify artworks whose appeal is in stark contrast to what we might expect, purely based on visual processing.

This could help expose a whole new area to explore to more fully appreciate what makes an artwork aesthetically appealing.

It’s possible that the lack of unusual spatial alignments in generic viewpoints help our brains to easily separate foreground from background objects, and those visual cues that make processing the world a bit easier are also the things we find appealing.

While our research doesn’t provide a ‘formula’ for creating good art, it does provide an insight into how certain aspects of what we find appealing are closely linked to fundamental aspects of how the eye and brain process visual information.

While it is unlikely that Monet’s The Lion Rock would’ve been more appealing if he had been a couple of centimetres shorter, it might well have been more appealing had he moved further down the hill.

Better still, though, appears to be his decision to turn his easel towards completely different sections of the coastline.

Banner: Rocks at Port Coton, The Lion Rock, Claude Monet (1840 – 1926) Oil on canvas/Fitzwilliam Museum

First published on 17 June 2022 in Health & Medicine

Optometry and Vision Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, Dentistry and Health Sciences, University of Melbourne

We believe in the free flow of information. This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-No Derivatives 3.0 Australia (CC BY-ND 3.0 AU), so you can republish our articles for free, online or in print.

All republished articles must be attributed in the following way and contain links to both the site and original article: “This article was first published on Pursuit. Read the original article.”

What if Monet were shorter? Associate Professor Andrew Anderson While visiting the Fitzwilliam Museum in Cambridge a few years ago, I happened upon the French Impressionist Claude Monet’s Rocks at Port Coton, The Lion Rock. Painted during his 1886 visit to the French island of Belle-Île, the work depicts the famous Lion Rock, sitting in the water below a cliff whose top perfectly aligns with the distant horizon. I didn’t like it. But I also had a good idea as to why I didn’t like it.Claude Monet’s Rocks at Port Coton, The Lion Rock alongside his other works at the Fitzwilliam Museum. Picture: Getty Images In 1999, influential neuroscientist Professor V. S. Ramachandran and American philosopher Professor William Hirstein proposed several principles they believed underpinned people’s aesthetic preferences in images. One principle was that people prefer generic viewpoints, where the spatial relationship between objects is largely similar despite small changes in the viewer’s position. For example, in the cartoon on the left in Figure 1 below, the foreground tree would appear somewhere between the peak and valley of the background hills even if the observer moved a little to the left or the right.Figure 1: Generic (left) vs Specific (right) viewpoints. Graphic: Anderson et al, 2022 So, the general composition of the work is maintained despite small alterations in our viewpoint. But if we look at the image on the right in Figure 1, the alignment between the foreground tree and the valley between the background hills would occur only from a single, specific viewpoint – so if the image were created with the observer moved a little to the left or the right, this alignment would be broken. In general, generic viewpoints are more common, making the spatial coincidences arising from specific viewpoints relatively unlikely. It is argued that these kind of spatial coincidences are seen by the brain as suspicious given their unlikely nature and, as a result, are unappealing. The remarkable journey of Leonardo's inscrutable masterpieceRead more In the case of foreground and background objects, it’s possible that the parts of the brain processing this visual information need to exert additional effort to ensure that a spatial coincidence is just that – a chance finding – rather than indicating the background and foreground are somehow physically connected. In the case of the Monet image, the coincidence between the clifftop and the horizon likely makes my brain need a few extra moments to decide that the distant cliff isn’t physically continuous with the horizon, but merely appears so from this particular vantage point. Over the 20 years since Ramachandran and Hirstein proposed that generic views are more aesthetically appealing, their work has been highly cited. Indeed, their paper is in Ramachandran’s top five most cited works, which is impressive given he is a particularly highly cited neuroscientist.Figure 2: Monet’s Rocks at Port Coton, The Lion Rock, including a version (right) manipulated to give a more generic view. Graphic: Anderson et al, 2022 Despite this, experimental support for their idea is limited. Given this – and prompted by my encounter with Monet’s Lion Rock – a team of students from the Department of Optometry and Vision Sciences and I set out to test Ramachandran and Hirstein’s proposal. We had people view a range of image pairs containing both specific and generic viewpoints between foreground and background objects and we asked them to choose which image they liked more. Our results found strong experimental support for the idea that people find generic viewpoints more appealing. Is this the earliest depiction of a dodo in art?Read more This includes when people are presented with a Monet image manipulated to give a more generic view (Figure 2). This would have been the result if Monet were (substantially) shorter – or, more realistically, if he had found a vantage point with a different elevation. Of course, there is more to a beautiful work of art than simply the spatial relationship between foreground and background objects. Various factors influence what we find appealing and this is reflected in the substantial variation in responses between our experimental observers. While our manipulations improve the overall appeal of Monet’s Lion Rock relative to the original, most people still find this manipulated work less appealing than many other works painted during his Belle-Île visit (Figure 3). So really, the influence of the specific viewpoint in The Lion Rock is small compared to the sum of other factors influencing aesthetic appeal. Figure 3: Comparing Monet’s Rocks at Port Coton, The Lion Rock to other coastal scenes he painted during his Belle-Île visit. Graphic: Anderson et al, 2022 Indeed, Ramachandran and Hirstein outline a number of visual factors that they think influence what we find appealing – along with evolutionary rationales for why this might be so. Using careful experiments to quantify how important these factors are might then allow us to better appreciate what non-visual factors – like cultural factors – affect what we find appealing. Experiments like this might also help identify artworks whose appeal is in stark contrast to what we might expect, purely based on visual processing. How did a cockatoo reach 13th century Sicily?Read more This could help expose a whole new area to explore to more fully appreciate what makes an artwork aesthetically appealing. It’s possible that the lack of unusual spatial alignments in generic viewpoints help our brains to easily separate foreground from background objects, and those visual cues that make processing the world a bit easier are also the things we find appealing. While our research doesn’t provide a ‘formula’ for creating good art, it does provide an insight into how certain aspects of what we find appealing are closely linked to fundamental aspects of how the eye and brain process visual information. While it is unlikely that Monet’s The Lion Rock would’ve been more appealing if he had been a couple of centimetres shorter, it might well have been more appealing had he moved further down the hill. Better still, though, appears to be his decision to turn his easel towards completely different sections of the coastline. Banner: Rocks at Port Coton, The Lion Rock, Claude Monet (1840 – 1926) Oil on canvas/Fitzwilliam Museum

While visiting the Fitzwilliam Museum in Cambridge a few years ago, I happened upon the French Impressionist Claude Monet’s Rocks at Port Coton, The Lion Rock.

Painted during his 1886 visit to the French island of Belle-Île, the work depicts the famous Lion Rock, sitting in the water below a cliff whose top perfectly aligns with the distant horizon.

I didn’t like it. But I also had a good idea as to why I didn’t like it.

In 1999, influential neuroscientist Professor V. S. Ramachandran and American philosopher Professor William Hirstein proposed several principles they believed underpinned people’s aesthetic preferences in images.

One principle was that people prefer generic viewpoints, where the spatial relationship between objects is largely similar despite small changes in the viewer’s position.

For example, in the cartoon on the left in Figure 1 below, the foreground tree would appear somewhere between the peak and valley of the background hills even if the observer moved a little to the left or the right.

So, the general composition of the work is maintained despite small alterations in our viewpoint.

But if we look at the image on the right in Figure 1, the alignment between the foreground tree and the valley between the background hills would occur only from a single, specific viewpoint – so if the image were created with the observer moved a little to the left or the right, this alignment would be broken.

In general, generic viewpoints are more common, making the spatial coincidences arising from specific viewpoints relatively unlikely.

It is argued that these kind of spatial coincidences are seen by the brain as suspicious given their unlikely nature and, as a result, are unappealing.

In the case of foreground and background objects, it’s possible that the parts of the brain processing this visual information need to exert additional effort to ensure that a spatial coincidence is just that – a chance finding – rather than indicating the background and foreground are somehow physically connected.

In the case of the Monet image, the coincidence between the clifftop and the horizon likely makes my brain need a few extra moments to decide that the distant cliff isn’t physically continuous with the horizon, but merely appears so from this particular vantage point.

Over the 20 years since Ramachandran and Hirstein proposed that generic views are more aesthetically appealing, their work has been highly cited. Indeed, their paper is in Ramachandran’s top five most cited works, which is impressive given he is a particularly highly cited neuroscientist.

Despite this, experimental support for their idea is limited.

Given this – and prompted by my encounter with Monet’s Lion Rock – a team of students from the Department of Optometry and Vision Sciences and I set out to test Ramachandran and Hirstein’s proposal.

We had people view a range of image pairs containing both specific and generic viewpoints between foreground and background objects and we asked them to choose which image they liked more.

Our results found strong experimental support for the idea that people find generic viewpoints more appealing.

This includes when people are presented with a Monet image manipulated to give a more generic view (Figure 2). This would have been the result if Monet were (substantially) shorter – or, more realistically, if he had found a vantage point with a different elevation.

Of course, there is more to a beautiful work of art than simply the spatial relationship between foreground and background objects. Various factors influence what we find appealing and this is reflected in the substantial variation in responses between our experimental observers.

While our manipulations improve the overall appeal of Monet’s Lion Rock relative to the original, most people still find this manipulated work less appealing than many other works painted during his Belle-Île visit (Figure 3).

So really, the influence of the specific viewpoint in The Lion Rock is small compared to the sum of other factors influencing aesthetic appeal.

Indeed, Ramachandran and Hirstein outline a number of visual factors that they think influence what we find appealing – along with evolutionary rationales for why this might be so.

Using careful experiments to quantify how important these factors are might then allow us to better appreciate what non-visual factors – like cultural factors – affect what we find appealing.

Experiments like this might also help identify artworks whose appeal is in stark contrast to what we might expect, purely based on visual processing.

This could help expose a whole new area to explore to more fully appreciate what makes an artwork aesthetically appealing.

It’s possible that the lack of unusual spatial alignments in generic viewpoints help our brains to easily separate foreground from background objects, and those visual cues that make processing the world a bit easier are also the things we find appealing.

While our research doesn’t provide a ‘formula’ for creating good art, it does provide an insight into how certain aspects of what we find appealing are closely linked to fundamental aspects of how the eye and brain process visual information.

While it is unlikely that Monet’s The Lion Rock would’ve been more appealing if he had been a couple of centimetres shorter, it might well have been more appealing had he moved further down the hill.

Better still, though, appears to be his decision to turn his easel towards completely different sections of the coastline.

Banner: Rocks at Port Coton, The Lion Rock, Claude Monet (1840 – 1926) Oil on canvas/Fitzwilliam Museum

The Media Office is staffed from 8am–5pm Monday to Friday.

The University has a television and radio studio to facilitate live and prerecorded broadcast quality interviews with media. You can also Find an expert for commentary.

Receive your weekly email digest from Pursuit

Sorry, something went wrong. Please try again later.

Please enter a valid email address.

By subscribing, you agree to our privacy statement.

The University of Melbourne (University) collects, uses, handles and discloses personal information in accordance with the Privacy and Data Protection Act 2014 (Vic) (Act) and other applicable legislation.

This Privacy Statement relates only to the collection of personal information in relation to the Pursuit Website. Please refer to our Privacy Policy and Privacy Statement for the University of Melbourne Website for information in relation to the broader practices in relation to the collection, use handling and disclosure of personal information by the University.

“Personal information” is defined under the Act to mean information or an opinion (including information or an opinion forming part of a database), that is recorded in any form and whether true or not, about an individual whose identity is apparent, or can reasonably be ascertained, from the information or opinion, but does not include information of a kind to which the Health Records Act 2001(Vic) applies. 

Collection of Personal Information by the University in relation to Pursuit

The University may collect, store and handle personal information about you including but not limited to your name and email address for the sole purpose of allowing you to subscribe to Pursuit’s weekly digest of cutting-edge research findings and expert commentary.

The University would seek your prior written consent before using your personal information for any purpose other than that which is described above and before disclosing your personal information to any third party.

Access to Your Personal Information

You can access any personal information the University holds about you by contacting the University’s Privacy Officer at privacy-officer@unimelb.edu.au.